![]() |
||||||||||||||
Australia: The Land Where Time Began |
||||||||||||||
Stone Tool
Studies The question arises of whether
stone tools are a good
guide to connecting behaviours in different parts of the world, which
again arise from naming practices for tool types and the assemblages of
tools. A small number of artefacts that were deliberately selected from
among the finds at Liang Bua, which were published with the original
species description
(Morwood et
al., 2004),
became part of an argument that suggests the tools were too
“sophisticated” to have been made by any known hominin other than modern
humans (Martin et
al., 2006).
An error has been exposed in relying on drawings of a few artefacts that
were carefully selected, by detailed analysis of the artefacts from
Liang Bua (Moore et
al.,
2009), and of those from the earliest sites that have been found in
Flores that date to several hundreds of thousands of years older (Brumm
et
al., 2006).
According to Davidson over the past more than 40
years Australian archaeologists have been sceptical of typologies (see
Holdaway & Stern, 2004: 283-315)., which has resulted in artefacts at
the present are rarely illustrated in published reports (but see Smith,
2006). Stone tools were not mentioned in a particular synthesis of
Australian archaeohistory (Davidson, 1999). There are only 8 stone
artefacts illustrated in the latest synthesis (Hiscock, 2008). Davidson
claims the interpretations of Australian artefacts can be misleading,
particularly those which involve comparisons of Australian stone
industries with Old World Oldowan, Acheulean, Levallois, Mousterian,
Upper Palaeolithic sequence (OALMUP; Davidson, 2009). According to
Davidson aspects of stone tools from Australia have been compared with
the Oldowan (e.g., Toth, 1985); in western Queensland in particular,
hand axes have been found (see analysis in Moore, 2003) and the Northern
Territory, though they are not in any way related to the Acheulean
“tradition” spatially and chronologically (Davidson, 2002); a technique
that would be classified as Levallois by experienced analysts
(Sonneville-Bordes, 1986) was used in Australia (Moore, 2003), though
some of the artefacts that might be identified as Levallois points in
other parts of the world were made by a blade-making technique on
single-platform cores that have been called horsehoof cores (Binford &
O’Connell, 1984). The case for the Levallois and related techniques
being the fundamental technologies taken from Africa by modern humans to
the rest of the world is weakened by the equifinality involved (see
e.g., Foley & Lahr, 1997). The fact is that these artefacts were made on
horsehoof cores in 1974, these cores are otherwise said to be
characteristic of the earliest stone industries of Australia (Bowdler et
al., 1970) is said by
Davidson to further undermine the use of both “Levallois” points and
horsehoof cores as type fossils of early industries. A final point made
by Davidson is that it has been suggested that “blade” industries do not
occur in early assemblages from Australia (Mellars, 2006b), which is to
some extent confirmed by the details of the assemblage recovered from
Puritjarra (Smith, 2006). According to Davidson the whole question of
blades is more complicated than is generally said to be in the standard
literature of the Old World, with blades being found throughout the
sequence in Australia (Davidson, 2003). At Lake Mungo in particular,
conjoining flakes onto horsehoof cores, dating to more than 40,000 years
old, demonstrated that blades had been removed from the site (Shawcross,
1998). The percentage found at Puritjarra, 3.6 calculated by the author
based on the data in Smith, 2010, is very low for an assemblage from the
period of modern humans, though it is higher than the proportion at the
Kapthurin in Africa, from the Middle Pleistocene (Johnson & McBrearty,
2010). There has been a long history in Australia of
trying to fit stone artefacts into the framework that was established
for Europe and, and to a lesser extent, Africa, and each attempt either
failed or was only partially successful. Davidson suggests there are 2
explanations for this that contradict each other: either the OALMUP
sequence is actually a universal aspect of hominin and human progress,
according to which the Australian Aboriginal people just did not measure
up; or the situation in Australia indicates something that is
fundamental about the flaws that result from trying to write a narrative
of hominin and human archaeology that is based on 5 basic stages
covering a period of 2 million years. Examination of an attempt (Mellars, 2006b, 798-799)
to account for the inconsistency for the OALMUP sequence by
1)
A scarcity of
raw materials that were suitable;
2)
There was no
necessity for complex tools in economies in which emphasis was on marine
resources, and in which there was no need to prepare skins for clothing;
and
3)
Founder effects, drift and “progressive
loss in the complexity and loss of culture and patterns of technology”.
An assumption, that are based on this underlying
argument, about the continuity of traditions and the probability of
convergence on particular forms of products produced by stone flaking,
which Davidson suggests is something like this:
1)
Archaeologists
are able to recognise patterns in the products of flaking that vary
throughout time and space.
2)
In terms of the
intentional actions of the knappers, these patterns are meaningful.
3)
Products of
flaking that are close to each other in time display similarities.
4)
When major
changes take place in the products of flaking between
chronostratigraphic periods, a cultural change is indicated.
5)
Products of
flaking generally display more similarity between regions that are
adjacent then between regions that are distant, provided there is no
major variation from one region to another.
6)
When big
differences are apparent between products of flaking in adjacent
regions, it may be an indication of a lack of cultural connection
between them, provided there is no substantial difference in raw
materials.
7)
The assumption that is required for of
this to be true is that the methods used in the making of such flaking
products were followed by succeeding generations. According to Davidson this set of assumptions has
dominated European thought of the subject from the 19th
century, though in Australia the ethnographic record does not provide
strong support for archaeologically defined types being of importance
for the people making and using flaked stone artefacts (Holdaway &
Douglas, 2012). Some of these propositions appear to be consistent with
sites that are stratified, though without the stratigraphic evidence,
inference becomes much more problematic. There are 2 different patterns
of bifacial flaking well-illustrate by some anomalies. Bifacial flaking
had similar end results in the Acheulean and Mousterian of Acheulean
Tradition that were chronologically separated in the first of these
cases, as well as in Australia at a much later time. That the particular
outcome may not result from tradition at all is suggested by
chronological and spatial separation, instead being an end product of
particular strategies of knapping. It is suggested by the evidence in
Australia that rather than being a continuous tradition, that “handaxes”
that are the products of bifacial knapping, could possibly have been
invented or discovered independently thousands of times. At later dates it is not likely there was any
connection through a common tradition of bifacially flaked points (for
the Solutrean and Clovis, see, e.g., Shott, 2013; Straus et
al., 2005). Davidson suggests
the most likely interpretation is that the points that were bifacially
flaked are also examples of people arriving at a solution to particular
needs quite independently by using a range of similar techniques.
Davidson suggests these points that are bifacially flaked are also
examples of people using a range of similar techniques which arrive at a
solution to particular needs independently, is the most likely
interpretation. Davidson suggests pieces that are apparently
complex such as large bifacial cores, as in the Acheulean or smaller
bifacial points, may have been invented or discovered independently more
than once, then they may have been invented many, many times, and the
same would apply to other “types”, as for example backed artefacts (contra
Mellars, 2006b) where it can be quite complex deciding what is to be
included in the comparison (Hiscock & O’Connor, 2005). Though narratives
depending almost entirely on assumptions that are not stated or not
examined, archaeologists have a propensity to see connect ion where
there is similarity. In spite of appearances, continuity of cultural
tradition is weaker than it has been assumed to have been; given the
relatively small number of options in knapping, it is to be expected
there would be similar outcomes (Moore, 2013). Claiming that
similarities provide an adequate basis for showing cultural connection
over long distances or long periods of time have been misleading. Also,
writing a cultural history in terms of “failure” to achieve the goals of
an illusory progress, as defined inductively based on the record in
Europe, is not licenced by a lack of similarity. What is to
be explained?
According to Davidson, the picture emerges of
people,
Homo sapiens, arriving in
Sahul, possibly by multiple routes, at sometime around 50,000 and 45,000
years ago, in spite of all the reservations archaeologists have
concerning the sources of evidence. They must have modern cognitive
abilities, as evidenced by their arrival on the continent of Sahul. The
use of symbols was one of those abilities, an ability which became a
dominant behavioural feature once in Sahul. The case has been made that
rock art was established at a relatively early time, and also that
personal ornaments may also have been used (Balme et
al., 2009; Mulvaney, 2013).
The early symbolic evidence has recently been analysed, the results
showing that within regions the use of symbol was patterned, and was
different between regions (Habgood & Franklin, 2011). Davidson says one of the functions of
archaeohistory is to provide an account of how the conditions that are
recorded historically emerged in a past that was not recorded (e.g.,
Davidson, 2010b). One question is what was the variation at the time of
the historical record that needs to be accounted for?
The other question is what were the processes of introduction,
diffusion or independent invention that contributed to the variation
that was recorded historically?
|
|
|||||||||||||
|
||||||||||||||
Author: M.H.Monroe Email: admin@austhrutime.com Sources & Further reading |